New! Sign up for our free email newsletter.
Science News
from research organizations

Naming guides how 12-month-old infants encode and remember objects

Encoding objects in memory and recalling them later is fundamental to human cognition

Date:
August 18, 2020
Source:
Northwestern University
Summary:
Even for infants just beginning to speak their first words, the way an object is named guides infants' encoding, representation and memory for that object, according to new research. Encoding objects in memory and recalling them later is fundamental to human cognition and emerges in infancy. Evidence from a new recognition memory task reveals that as they encode objects, infants are sensitive to a principled link between naming and object representation by 12 months.
Share:
FULL STORY

Even for infants just beginning to speak their first words, the way an object is named guides infants' encoding, representation and memory for that object, according to new Northwestern University research.

Encoding objects in memory and recalling them later is fundamental to human cognition and emerges in infancy.

Evidence from a new recognition memory task reveals that as they encode objects, infants are sensitive to a principled link between naming and object representation by 12 months.

During training, all infants viewed four distinct objects from the same object category, each introduced in conjunction with either the same novel noun (Consistent Name condition), a distinct novel noun for each object (Distinct Names condition), or the same sine-wave tone sequence (Consistent Tone condition).

Researchers then tested whether infants remembered which objects they had just seen during training. To do so, infants viewed each training object again, this time presented in silence along with a new object from the same object category. Infants' memory for the individual objects was sensitive to how they had been named. Infants in the Consistent Name condition showed poor recognition memory at test, suggesting that consistently applied names focused them primarily on commonalities among the named objects at the expense of distinctions among them. In contrast, infants in the Distinct Names condition recognized three of the four objects, suggesting that applying distinct names enhanced infants' encoding of the distinctions among the individual objects. Infants in the control Consistent Tone condition recognized only the object they had most recently seen.

"We show that the way an object is named, either as a member of a category, for example, "This is a dog" or as a unique individual, for example, "This is Fido," is instrumental in 12-month-old infants' encoding of and memory for that object," said Alexander S. LaTourrette, co-author of the study. "When the same name is applied consistently to a set of objects, infants encode primarily their commonalties. In contrast, when a unique name is applied to each object, infants encode each object's unique features."

Sandra Waxman, co-author of the study and the Lewis W. Menk professor of psychology in the Weinberg College of Arts and Sciences, said this provides the first demonstration that for infants as young as 12 months of age, whether and how an object is named has rapid and conceptually precise consequences on infants' representation of that object.

"Moreover, the precision of infants' responses reveal that naming objects, even a single naming episode, can have a lasting impact on how infants encode that object, represent it in memory and remember it later," said Waxman, a faculty fellow with the University's Institute for Policy Research.

The researchers said this work sheds new light on the powerful and well-documented advantage of naming on infant object categorization, leaving little doubt that naming a set of distinct individual objects with the same noun invites infants to form an object category.

"Here, we show that hearing the same name for objects invites infants to focus on the commonalities among the objects, but at the expense of remembering the features that are unique to each individual," LaTourrette said.

Waxman said the new evidence opens new avenues for future investigations.

"First, because the current experiment focused exclusively on novel nouns, it remains an open question how familiar nouns, for example, 'Fido' or 'dog,' influence infants' object representations," she said.

"Addressing this question should also shed light on current debates concerning the link between language and cognition in adults. Second, it will be important to specify the effects of naming on infants' object representations throughout the first year of life. We know that naming supports the establishment of object categories consistently throughout the first year, beginning as early as 3 months of age. What remains unknown is whether naming confers its benefits via the same mechanisms throughout this period."


Story Source:

Materials provided by Northwestern University. Original written by Hilary Hurd Anyaso. Note: Content may be edited for style and length.


Journal Reference:

  1. Alexander S. LaTourrette, Sandra R. Waxman. Naming guides how 12-month-old infants encode and remember objects. Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences, 2020; 202006608 DOI: 10.1073/pnas.2006608117

Cite This Page:

Northwestern University. "Naming guides how 12-month-old infants encode and remember objects." ScienceDaily. ScienceDaily, 18 August 2020. <www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2020/08/200818142113.htm>.
Northwestern University. (2020, August 18). Naming guides how 12-month-old infants encode and remember objects. ScienceDaily. Retrieved October 31, 2024 from www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2020/08/200818142113.htm
Northwestern University. "Naming guides how 12-month-old infants encode and remember objects." ScienceDaily. www.sciencedaily.com/releases/2020/08/200818142113.htm (accessed October 31, 2024).

Explore More

from ScienceDaily

RELATED STORIES